Skip to main content
Log in

The economic effects of Facebook

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Experimental Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Social media permeates many aspects of our lives, including how we connect with others, where we get our news and how we spend our time. Yet, we know little about the economic effects for users. In 2017, we ran a large field experiment with over 1765 individuals to document the value of Facebook to users and its causal effect on news, well-being and daily activities. Participants reveal how much they value one week of Facebook usage and are then randomly assigned to a validated Facebook restriction or normal use. One week of Facebook is worth $67. Those who are off Facebook for one week reduce news consumption, are less likely to recognize politically-skewed news stories, report being less depressed and engage in healthier activities. These results are strongest for men. Our results further suggest that, after the restriction, Facebook’s value increases, consistent with information loss or that using Facebook may be addictive.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is estimated that the impact of Facebook through advertising is $77.6 billion in the U.S. (Deloitte 2015). Evidence on the value of Facebook is given in Brynjolfsson et al. (2018).

  2. We note that the BDM mechanism used in our study, and in other studies using the BDM or other mechanisms, involve hassle costs and some complexity that may affect values. Our participants face a one in two chance of experiencing a Facebook restriction, and this may reduce bias in value estimates when using elicitation mechanisms coupled with implementation uncertainty.

  3. Tromholt (2016) uses a one-week, self-enforced Facebook restriction and finds a positive effect on overall life satisfaction.

  4. The study is registered in the AEA Registry (AEARCTR-0003952)

  5. The email text and online survey questions are in the Appendix, Sections A.1 & A.2 in ESM

  6. Our procedures made clear to participants that they would be paid the random offer upon study completion to mitigate any uncertainty bias (Horowitz 2006).

  7. For budgetary reasons and expected participation rates, the random counter offers were drawn with the following probabilities: (5, 15.14%; 7, 15.14%; 9, 11.14%; 10, 11.14%; 12, 11.14%; 14, 11.14%; 16, 7.14%; 18, 6.14%; 20, 5.14%; 21, 5.14%; 24, 0.64%; 25, 0.64%; 28, 0.14%; 30, 0.14%). The expected offer is $11.58.

  8. Those who are ineligible for subsequent phases are not contacted.

  9. Participants complete the survey in Phase 2, prior to random assignment to the Facebook restriction, and in Phase 3. One might be concerned that changes in outcomes are due to experimenter demand effects. First, participants are not aware they will complete the same survey questions a second time. Second, we find effects for some, but not most, of the outcomes, thus alleviating concerns of such an effect. Finally, in a later study with a similar design, Allcott et al. (2019) find similar results to ours while explicitly testing for demand effects.

  10. We chose Breitbart given that its internet traffic as of March 2017 surpassed other major skewed news sources and was similar in magnitude to that of mainstream news sources such as The Washington Post according to data from alexa.com

  11. See the questionnaire in Appendix A.3 in ESM

  12. The university randomly generates the last four digits of a student’s ID number.

  13. We updated the news pool to reflect headlines from the previous week.

  14. Participants are asked to write down their valuation and informed that their payment today is unaffected by their response. Eligible participants from this second BDM go through the same process as in Phase 2, return for a third and final survey in one week, and are paid their counteroffers from the second BDM. We do not include this third survey in our estimates.

  15. Eligibility for Phase 2 means that the submitted value was less than a randomly-selected counter-offer of no more than $30. This is by the design of the elicitation mechanism—so all those with submitted values higher than $30 were ineligible. Descriptive statistics for these groups are in Appendix Table A.5.1 in ESM. In Sect. 4.1 of the paper, we test the robustness of the results to this design-induced selection.

  16. When we compare participants who submitted values less than or equal to $30, so they could have been eligible to participate in Phase 2, there is no significant difference by age or gender between those who ended up being eligible or ineligible based on the counteroffer. See Appendix Table A.5.1 in ESM

  17. Participants were aware of this procedure prior to submitting their value of Facebook in the Phase 1 online survey. Holding this session on main campus minimizes travel costs that might have affected valuations for Facebook.

  18. Appendix Table A.5.2 in ESM shows the balance of covariates across the treatment and control groups.

  19. Participants did not interact with the study account in any way.

  20. Appendix Table A.5.3 in ESM

  21. Appendix Table A.5.3 in ESM. While we cannot say what proportion of news participants get from Facebook, 81% report opening up Facebook every day or several times a day to check their news feed.

  22. We use the rankings on www.allsides.com. If a participant lists a news outlet that is not reported on allsides.com, we treat their preferred news outlet as missing. The top five first choice sources are CNN (28.1%), FOX (12.6%), BBC (8.3%), NYT (4.7%), and ESPN (4.7%). Breitbart was not listed as a first choice, however, news from this source could appear on a Facebook news feed.

  23. Appendix Figure A.5.1 in ESM shows the distribution of these emotions separately.

  24. We calculate the confidence intervals using bootstrap with 1000 replications.

  25. Our design also explored the willingness to pay (WTP)—willingness to accept (WTA) gap in the BDM mechanism [see Knetsch et al. (2001), Plott and Zeiler (2005), Horowitz (2006), and Brynjolfsson et al. (2018) for a discussion of this phenomenon]. Half of the participants were asked the value in terms of selling participation in the study (WTA), “How much money would you need to be given to stop using Facebook for a week?” and half were asked in terms of purchasing participation (WTP), “What is the value of your weekly time on Facebook?” We find no significant difference in the reported value of Facebook from either solicitation method or by covariates across groups, so we pool the data in our analysis.

  26. The distribution is trimmed at $540 because of a few outliers in the data—the maximum value is $2,153. We use the nontrimmed, full sample in our analysis.

  27. There are differences across studies. Brynjolfsson et al. (2018) use an online sample, one out of every 200 participants are randomized into the Facebook restriction, and respondents who do not use Facebook are not screened out for their weekly estimate. Corrigan et al. (2018) use a series of second-price auctions with different samples and compensation schemes. Allcott et al. (2019) also use a BDM mechanism but with an online sample.

  28. Women are typically found to be more risk averse than men. The risk-adjusted values of Facebook that we use assume that men and women have the same level of risk aversion. Women would need to be 37% more risk-averse than men for the difference to be significant at the 10% level, 41% more risk-averse than men for the difference to be significant at the 5% level, and 48% more risk-averse than men for the difference to be significant at the 1% level.

  29. We did not ask questions on income but asked the zip code of where the participant lived at age 15. Using income data from this zip code, we find no significant difference in mean income for younger participants compared to older.

  30. Appendix Table A.5.4 in ESM presents the Pearson correlation coefficients between the value of Facebook and several measures that characterize Facebook users.

  31. In-state tuition at Texas A&M is $11,200 per year, or $350 per week, implying that participants value Facebook as much as 19% of the weekly cost of studying at the university. According to the College Board, the average university student in the U.S. spends $225 per week ($10,800 per year) on room and board. Facebook is then worth 30% of these expenses.

  32. All but three treated participants stayed off of Facebook for the entire week. The three who did log back into Facebook did so only once for less than an hour to communicate for a student organization via the organization’s Facebook account. All three participants contacted the research team prior to logging in to inform us why they were logging back on. These participants are included in our intent-to-treat analysis. Instrumental variable estimates are 5% larger and slightly less precise.

  33. It would be important to test for effects at different quantiles, but we do not have enough power to estimate meaningful comparisons at the tails of the distribution. To test for distribution equality, let \(F_{(1)}\) be the distribution of outcome \(y_{it}\) for the treated group and \(F_{(0)}\) be the distribution of the control group. According to Abadie (2002), we define \(F_{(1)}\) first order stochastic dominates \(F_{(0)}\) if \(\int _0^x dF_{(1)}(y) \le \int _0^x dF_{(0)}(y) \ \forall x \ge 0\) and \(F_{(1)}\) second order stochastic dominates \(F_{(0)}\) if \(\int _0^x ( \int _0^z dF_{(1)}(y) ) dz \le \int _0^x ( \int _0^z dF_{(0)}(y) ) dz \ \forall x \ge 0\)

  34. Doing this involves a trade off between a Type I error and the power of the test (Anderson 2008). We control for the false discovery rate to adjust our p values and achieve a balance between these two factors.

  35. For reference, both the unadjusted and adjusted p values are reported in Table A.5.6. All of our results remain statistically significant at the 5% level or less, with the exception of the probability of answering “Don’t Know” for skewed news, the healthy activities index and the change in the value of Facebook. We also do a more robust adjustment controlling for the family-wise error rate. When we use the free step-down method described by Anderson (2008), only the effects on Facebook use, news access through social media, news consumption and the correct answer of skewed news are statistically significant at conventional levels.

  36. We tested whether the Facebook restriction had different effects for true headlines and the false headlines we created (by changing a few words) in the news quiz. For both types of headlines, the point estimates are similar to the main results and statistically insignificant.

  37. Gender differences do emerge. While both men and women are less likely to be aware of the veracity of skewed news when off of Facebook, the effect is much stronger for men than women. This suggest that men, more than women, are exposed to politically skewed news when on Facebook.

  38. Our research design restricted usage of Facebook for those in the treatment group, but participants were not restricted in their usage of other social media platforms. We validate that those in the treatment group did reduce their use of Facebook—by 1.7 h per day. Given a baseline Facebook usage of 1.9 h per day, this illustrates that the treatment group did comply with the restriction. While the treatment group refrained from using Facebook, we find that they did not increase their usage of other social media (e.g. Instagram, Snapchat, Tumblr, Twitter). This is consistent with studies finding low cross-platform usage for social media and a significant cost to switch to alternatives for one week (Pew Research Center 2016). Only one-third of Facebook users are active on other social media platforms, yet about 90% of users of other platforms are active on Facebook (Pew Research Center 2016).

  39. The questions are: (1) Overall, how satisfied are you with life as a whole? (2) Overall, to what extent do you feel that things you do in your life are worthwhile? (3) How happy are you? (4) How often do you worry? and (5) How often do you feel depressed? An alternative approach could have been to use the Day Reconstruction Method (Kahneman et al. 2004; Kahneman and Krueger 2006), however, to keep the survey short, we opted for the five OECD questions.

  40. Our results on life satisfaction are smaller than Tromholt (2016) who finds a significant effect of 0.26 standard deviations. The study’s Danish sample is older (average age of 34 years) compared to our U.S. sample (average age of 20 years), and participants were contacted daily by the researcher team to follow their assigned treatment status.

  41. Subjective well-being measures can be sensitive to temporary events (e.g. the weather, long lines at a coffee shop, meeting somebody) (Krueger and Schkade 2008), nonetheless, because our participants are randomly assigned to treatment, random shocks should be evenly distributed and our panel estimation allows us to directly control for events that affect both groups uniformly across time.

  42. There is evidence that eating out is associated with excessive calorie intake (Urban et al. 2016), a less healthy diet (Wolfson and Bleich 2015), increased hypertension (Seow et al. 2015) and a higher exposure to phthalates (Varshavsky et al. 2018), which have been linked to asthma, breast cancer, type 2 diabetes and fertility issues. Diet is correlated with an individual’s mental health (O’Neil et al. 2014).

  43. Participants were asked on a scale 1–5 to what extent they agreed with a particular statement, where 1: Strongly Agree, 5: Strongly Disagree. We adjust the coding so a higher value indicates a “healthier” response.

  44. The adjusted p value is 0.125. Our sample size allows us to detect effects up to 0.182 percentage points at the 5% level with a power of 80%.

  45. A key characteristic of an addictive good is that its consumption exhibits “adjacent complementarity” (Becker and Murphy 1988; Gruber and Köszegi 2001), which means that past consumption increases the marginal utility of present consumption.

  46. Appendix Figure A.5.2 in ESM shows that while the level of depression in the treatment group has decreased relative to control group, there is no evidence that suggests that treated participants are internalizing this benefit by lowering their value for Facebook.

  47. In our sample, about 43% of women post comments on Facebook at least once a week, compared to 21% of men. Also, 23% of women post pictures at least once a week compared to 8% of men.

References

  • Abadie, A. (2002). Bootstrap tests for distributional treatment effects in instrumental variable models. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 97(457), 284–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allcott, H., Braghieri, L., Eichmeyer, S., & Gentzkow, M. (2019). The welfare effects of social media. Technical report, National Bureau of Economic Research.

  • Allcott, H., & Gentzkow, M. (2017). Social media and fake news in the 2016 election. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 31(2), 211–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. L. (2008). Multiple inference and gender differences in the effects of early intervention: A reevaluation of the Abecedarian, Perry Preschool, and Early Training Projects. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 103(484), 1481–1495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashley, C., & Tuten, T. (2015). Creative strategies in social media marketing: An exploratory study of branded social content and consumer engagement. Psychology & Marketing, 32(1), 15–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, M., Cao, R., Kuchler, T., & Stroebel, J. (2017). The economic effects of social networks: Evidence from the housing market. Journal of Political Economy, 126(6), 2224–2276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, M., Cao, R., Kuchler, T., Stroebel, J., & Wong, A. (2018). Social connectedness: Measurement, determinants, and effects. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32(3), 259–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. M., Degroot, M. H., & Marschak, J. (1964). Measuring utility by a single-response sequential method. Behavioral Science, 9(3), 226–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S., & Murphy, K. M. (1988). A theory of rational addiction. Journal of Political Economy, 96(4), 675–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y., Krieger, A. M., & Yekutieli, D. (2006). Adaptive linear step-up procedures that control the false discovery rate. Biometrika, 93(3), 491–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blake, H., Mo, P., Malik, S., & Thomas, S. (2009). How effective are physical activity interventions for alleviating depressive symptoms in older people? A systematic review. Clinical Rehabilitation, 23(10), 873–887.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohm, P., Lindén, J., & Sonnegåd, J. (1997). Eliciting reservation prices: Becker–Degroot–Marschak mechanisms versus markets. The Economic Journal, 107(443), 1079–1089.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, R. M., Fariss, C. J., Jones, J. J., Kramer, A. D., Marlow, C., Settle, J. E., et al. (2012). A 61-million-person experiment in social influence and political mobilization. Nature, 489(7415), 295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brynjolfsson, E., Eggers, F., & Gannamaneni, A. (2018). Using massive online chohice experiments to measure changes in well-being. NBER Working Paper 24514.

  • Corrigan, J. R., Alhabash, S., Rousu, M., & Cash, S. B. (2018). How much is social media worth? Estimating the value of Facebook by paying users to stop using it. PloS One, 13(12), e0207101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, S., & Inkster, B. (2017). Status of mind social media and young people’s mental health and wellbeing. Royal Society for Public Health.

  • De Vries, L., Gensler, S., & Leeflang, P. S. (2012). Popularity of brand posts on brand fan pages: An investigation of the effects of social media marketing. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 26(2), 83–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • DellaVigna, S., & Kaplan, E. (2007). The fox news effect: Media bias and voting. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122(3), 1187–1234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deloitte. (2015). Facebook’s global economic impact: A report for facebook.

  • Deters, Fg, & Mehl, M. R. (2013). Does posting facebook status updates increase or decrease loneliness? An online social networking experiment. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(5), 579–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of facebook ”friends”: Social capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12(4), 1143–1168.

    Google Scholar 

  • EMarketeer. (2015). College students still spend most social time with facebook. Retrieved December 25, 2017 from https://www.emarketer.com/Article/College-Students-Still-Spend-Most-Social-Time-with-Facebook/1012955.

  • Enikolopov, R., Makarin, A., & Petrova, M. (2016). Social media and protest participation: Evidence from Russia. Universitat Pompeu Fabra.

  • Facebook. (2016). Facebook reports first quarter 2016 results.

  • Facebook. (2017). Facebook reports third quarter 2017 results.

  • Feinstein, B. A., Hershenberg, R., Bhatia, V., Latack, J. A., Meuwly, N., & Davila, J. (2013). Negative social comparison on facebook and depressive symptoms: Rumination as a mechanism. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2(3), 161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flood, S., King, M., Ruggles, S., & Warren, J. R. (2017). Integrated public use microdata series, current population survey: Version 5.0. dataset, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN. https://doi.org/10.18128/D030.V5.0.

  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13(2), 172–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentzkow, M., & Shapiro, J. M. (2011). Ideological segregation online and offline. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126(4), 1799–1839.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzales, A. L., & Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my facebook wall: Effects of exposure to facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(1–2), 79–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, J., & Shearer, E. (2016). News use across social media platforms 2016.

  • Gruber, J., & Köszegi, B. (2001). Is addiction ”Rational”? Theory and evidence. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(4), 1261–1303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herzog, B. (2018). Valuation of digital platforms: Experimental evidence for google and facebook. International Journal of Financial Studies, 6(4), 87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, J. K. (2006). The Becker–DeGroot–Marschak mechanism is not necessarily incentive compatible, even for non-random goods. Economics Letters, 93(1), 6–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jelenchick, L. A., Eickho, J. C., & Moreno, M. A. (2013). Facebook depression? Social networking site use and depression in older adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52(1), 128–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. B. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306(5702), 1776–1780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kettunen, O. (2015). Effects of physical activity and fitness on the psychological wellbeing of young men and working adults: Associations with stress, mental resources, overweight and workability. Turku: Annales universitas turkuensis, medica-odontologica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., & Lee, J. (2011). The facebook paths to happiness: Effects of the number of facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. Cyber Psychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(6), 359–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knetsch, J. L., Tang, F.-F., & Thaler, R. H. (2001). The endowment effect and repeated market trials: Is the Vickrey auction demand revealing? Experimental Economics, 4(3), 257–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, A. B., & Schkade, D. A. (2008). The reliability of subjective well-being measures. Journal of Public Economics, 92(8), 1833–1845.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laroche, M., Habibi, M. R., Richard, M.-O., & Sankaranarayanan, R. (2012). The effects of social media based brand communities on brand community markers, value creation practices, brand trust and brand loyalty. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(5), 1755–1767.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, C. S., & Ma, L. (2012). News sharing in social media: The effect of gratifications and prior experience. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(2), 331–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, G. J., & Yurukoglu, A. (2017). Bias in cable news: Persuasion and polarization. American Economic Review, 107(9), 2565–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, A., & Puller, S. L. (2008). The old boy (and girl) network: Social network formation on university campuses. Journal of Public Economics, 92(1), 329–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazar, N., Koszegi, B., & Ariely, D. (2014). True context-dependent preferences? The causes of market-dependent valuations. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 27(3), 200–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullainathan, S., & Shleifer, A. (2005). The market for news. American Economic Review, 95(4), 1031–1053.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muscanell, N. L., & Guadagno, R. E. (2012). Make new friends or keep the old: Gender and personality differences in social networking use. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(1), 107–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, D. B., Tay, L., & Diener, E. (2014). Leisure and subjective well-being: A model of psychological mechanisms as mediating factors. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15(3), 555–578.

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD Better Life Initiative. (2013). OECD guidelines on measuring subjective well-being.

  • Ostir, G. V., Markides, K. S., Black, S. A., & Goodwin, J. S. (2000). Emotional well-being predicts subsequent functional independence and survival. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48(5), 473–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Neil, A., Quirk, S. E., Housden, S., Brennan, S. L., Williams, L. J., Pasco, J. A., et al. (2014). Relationship between diet and mental health in children and adolescents: A systematic review. American Journal of Public Health, 104(10), e31–e42. PMID: 25208008.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew Research Center. (2016). Social media update 2016.

  • Pew Research Center. (2018). Social media use 2018.

  • Plott, C. R., & Zeiler, K. (2005). The willingness to pay-willingness to accept gap, the ”endowment effect”, subject misconceptions, and experimental procedures for eliciting valuations. The American Economic Review, 95(3), 530–545.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rezaee, M., Hedayati, A., Naghizadeh, M. M., Farjam, M., Sabet, H. R., & Paknahad, M. (2016). Correlation between happiness and depression according to beck depression and oxford happiness inventory among university students. Galen Medical Journal, 5(2), 75–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sagioglou, C., & Greitemeyer, T. (2014). Facebook’s emotional consequences: Why facebook causes a decrease in mood and why people still use it? Computers in Human Behavior, 35, 359–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salovey, P., Rothman, A. J., Detweiler, J. B., & Steward, W. T. (2000). Emotional states and physical health. American Psychologist, 55(1), 110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seow, D. Y., Haaland, B., & Jafar, T. H. (2015). The association of prehypertension with meals eaten away from home in young adults in singapore. American Journal of Hypertension, 28(10), 1197–1200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shakya, H. B., & Christakis, N. A. (2017). Association of facebook use with compromised well-being: A longitudinal study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 185(3), 203–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonnentag, S. (2001). Work, recovery activities, and individual well-being: A diary study. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6(3), 196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Statista. (2018). Frequency of facebook use in the united states as of february 2018 by gender. Retrieved June 23, 2018 from https://www.statista.com/statistics/653014/frequency-usage-facebook-usa-gender/.

  • Steers, M.-L. N., Wickham, R. E., & Acitelli, L. K. (2014). Seeing everyone else’s highlight reels: How facebook usage is linked to depressive symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 33(8), 701–731.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunstein, C. R. (2019). Valuing Facebook. Behavioural Public Policy. https://doi.org/10.1017/bpp.2018.34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tandoc, E. C., Ferrucci, P., & Duy, M. (2015). Facebook use, envy, and depression among college students: Is facebooking depressing? Computers in Human Behavior, 43, 139–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • The Neilsen Company. (2016). 2016 neilsen social media report. The Neilsen Company.

  • Tromholt, M. (2016). The facebook experiment: Quitting facebook leads to higher levels of well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 19(11), 661–666.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuttle, B. (2012). How much you spend each year on Coffee, Gas, Christmas, Pets, Beer, and More. TIME Magazine. Retrieved December 25, 2017 from http://business.time.com/2012/01/23/how-much-you-spend-each-year-on-coffee-gas-christmas-pets-beer-and-more/.

  • Urban, L. E., Weber, J. L., Heyman, M. B., Schichtl, R. L., Verstraete, S., Lowery, N. S., et al. (2016). Energy contents of frequently ordered restaurant meals and comparison with human energy requirements and us department of agriculture database information: A multisite randomized study. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 116(4), 590–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valenzuela, S., Park, N., & Kee, K. F. (2009). Is there social capital in a social network site?: Facebook use and college students’ life satisfaction, trust, and participation. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14(4), 875–901.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varshavsky, J. R., Morello-Frosch, R., Woodruff, T. J., & Zota, A. R. (2018). Dietary sources of cumulative phthalates exposure among the us general population in nhanes 2005–2014. Environment International, 115, 417–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., et al. (2015). Passive facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, E. A., Rose, J. P., Okdie, B. M., Eckles, K., & Franz, B. (2015). Who compares and despairs? The effect of social comparison orientation on social media use and its outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 249–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfson, J. A., & Bleich, S. N. (2015). Is cooking at home associated with better diet quality or weight-loss intention? Public Health Nutrition, 18(8), 1397–1406.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ragan Petrie.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (pdf 2644 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mosquera, R., Odunowo, M., McNamara, T. et al. The economic effects of Facebook. Exp Econ 23, 575–602 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-019-09625-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-019-09625-y

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation